Indicators of Acculturation: A Bilinear, Multidimensional Approach

By Dina Drankus

News Type
Advocates' Forum

Abstract

This paper presents an instrument for assessing a client’s level of acculturation. The instrument has been designed for use by social work clinicians working with clients that affiliate or identify with a culture of origin that differs from mainstream culture in theUnited States. This bilinear, multidimensional instrument is composed of thirty indicators organized into three domains: language, cultural behavior, and cultural knowledge.  Existing research that informed the domain selection is reviewed and the rationale behind the inclusion of each indicator is given. The instrument is presented in such a way as to show how it should be adapted or augmented as needed by practitioners in their work with clients. The paper shows how instrument is designed to: (1) increase practitioner understanding of a client’s degree of acculturation; (2) increase the practitioner’s cultural awareness and sensitivity, and (3) increase the practitioner’s recognition of the impact of acculturation on other health, psychological, and behavioral outcomes and health and social service utilization.


Culture consists of the learned symbols, language, behavior, tradition, and ideas that are distinct among different groups of people. Culture is dynamic and evolves over time and change can be expedited by major events or as groups come in contact with each other. Individual acculturation refers to that complex, dynamic process of adaptation that takes place when one interacts with a dominant culture that differs from his or her heritage culture. The traditional definition comes from Redfield, Linton, and Herskovits (1936, 149): “Acculturation comprehends those phenomena which result when groups of individuals having different cultures come into continuous first-hand contact, with subsequent changes in the original culture patterns of either or both groups.” This definition, however, is very broad and has left the assessment of individual- and group-levels of acculturation open to a multitude of conceptualizations and operationalizations.  Due to these multiple approaches to the process, practitioners working with bicultural clients lack guidance in selecting an appropriate and effective method of assessing a client’s level of acculturation.

A definitive conceptualization of the broad term acculturation and the more specific construct of individual-level acculturation have been elusive as scholars differ on how to demarcate acculturation as a concept unique from other related concepts, such as ethnic identity or cultural change and how to understand the mechanisms of cultural adaptation.  Defining acculturation is further complicated by the ever-changing context in which acculturation occurs, due largely to the socially-constructed, dynamic nature of culture (Bennett 2005; Bennett, Bennett and Landis 2004; Bidney 1947; Handwerker 2002; Wolf 1984; Wolf 1982).

An Approach to Acculturation

As people are increasingly exposed to multiple cultures, practitioners will work with clients with varying degrees of acculturation to two or more cultures. Practitioners can therefore use an instrument for increasing their awareness of a client’s level of acculturation and the acculturative process. Ideally this will promote culturally competent practice.

While the concept of acculturation predicts impacts at both the cultural and individual levels, I am concentrating on the ways in which the acculturative process manifests itself in individual-level adaptive changes. Contact with the dominant culture impacts the individual and he will undergo changes that allow for his maneuvering in that dominant culture. I am further defining indicators of acculturation as those reasonably objective, reportable aspects of an individual’s adaptation that are to some degree removed from the individual’s cognitive processing and allow for assessment of an individual’s level of acculturation rather than how the individual has processed, or made sense of, his or her acculturative process. For example, asking how often an individual speaks English at work requires less reflection and less cognitive processing than asking an individual if he or she feels US-American. The level of reflection required to understand one’s identity is beyond the scope of the below acculturation instrument and, further, reflecting or processing one’s position or identity in the dominant culture does not actually indicate the level to which the individual has adopted aspects of the dominant culture. The goal of these indicators is to create an individual acculturation profile based on reported language, behaviors, and knowledge that can be compared to broader dominant cultural norms. The respondent is not asked to report perceived cultural differences or ethnic identity. 

The Construction of the Instrument

The instrument presented here is the cohesive combination of all the indicators of acculturation. It is multidimensional and bilinear.  Multidimensionality refers to assessing acculturation across multiple domains and bilinearity refers to the instrument’s ability to capture non-inverse relationships in the individual’s participation in the heritage and the dominant cultures (i.e., participation in one culture does not preclude participation in the other culture) (Zea, Asner-Self, Birman, and Buk 2003). It should be noted that bilinearity as I have defined it is often referred to as multidimensionality in the literature (Berry 1980; Berry 1997). In my presentation below, I will also refer to heritage and dominant cultures. Heritage culture is often referred to as the home culture or culture of origin in the literature. Dominant culture is often referred to as mainstreamUSculture, receiving culture, or host culture in the literature. Finally, I would like to recognize that many of the studies I reference differ in their conceptualization of acculturation and in their operationalization of indicators of acculturation, but these studies include features or outcomes that support or relate to my selection of indicators of acculturation.

Gordon’s (1964) model of unidirectional, unilinear assimilation shows a heritage culture permanently shed as an individual becomes more assimilated to the host culture. This may have been descriptive of the migrant assimilation experience in the late part of the 19th century and the early part of the 20th century, but migrants today do not necessarily cleave their home culture as they become acculturated to a new one. Acculturation today is most often a bilinear process that occurs across multiple domains, or dimensions, of an individual’s life. Such a complex process cannot be understood as unilinear. A bilinear approach that pairs indicators can better account for the high participation in behaviors related to a new culture without precluding the respondent’s high participation in behaviors related to her heritage culture. Similarly, the use of a bilinear instrument allows for a tracking of low participation in behaviors related to both a dominant and a heritage culture.

The bilinear approach offers additional benefits. Several studies comparing the use of unidimensional measures and bidimensional measures have found the use of bidimensional measures reveals non-inverse relationships between the dominant and heritage cultural orientations. Therefore it better captures relationships between acculturation, identity, and quality of life indicators (Abe-Kim, Okazaki and Goto 2001; Lee, Sobal and Frongillo 2003; Lieber, Chin, Nihira and Mink 2001; Nguyen, Messé and Stollak 1999;Ryder, Alden and Paulhaus 2000; Tsai 2001; Tsai, Ying and Lee 2000). Since bidimensional measures are more valid and useful in assessing a client’s level of acculturation, practitioners relying on unilinear indicators risk uncritically accepting the dominant ideology of the host country as the endpoint of the acculturative process and thus making normative and prescriptive statements to clients regarding their acculturative goals (Adrados 1997; Berry 2003).  The bilinear approach, therefore, refuses to problematize perceived non-acculturation or non-assimilation.

The importance of a bilinear approach in assessing level of acculturation is further maintained by studies that demonstrate the value of emotional or social support from the heritage culture in mediating acculturative stress and decreasing the likelihood of poor mental and physical health outcomes. The bilinear approach also offers the researcher the ability to distinguish between, or isolate, the impact of social support found in the dominant culture versus the support found in the heritage culture. These studies suggest that using a unilinear, rather than a bilinear, model of acculturation may miss the connection between a client’s ties with his or her heritage culture and healthy socio-psychological and physical functioning, as well as the nuances of an individual’s support system (Finch and Vega 2003; Lee, Crittenden and Yu 1996; Vega, Kolody, Valle and Weir 1991; Ward and Kennedy 1993).

The instrument’s indicators are organized into three domains: language, cultural behavior, and cultural knowledge. Language has long been recognized as a primary mechanism of cultural conveyance. The indicators within the cultural behavior and cultural knowledge domains act as both proxies and, in some cases, direct measures for client participation in social institutions in two ways. First, by measuring client consumption of mass media of the dominant and heritage cultures, assuming measures of consumption of English-language media is frequently equivalent to measures of client consumption of US mass media, several indicators directly measure participation in what I contend is the social institution of media (Silverblatt 2004). Second, by measuring client cultural knowledge of dominant and heritage cultures, several indicators indirectly measure contact with social institutions of both cultures, as I contend that contact with social institutions are frequently the mode of cultural knowledge transmission (Newman 2010). As stated by Bidney (1947, 375), culture is “communicated largely by language or symbolic forms and through participation in social institutions,” and I have focused my indicators on measuring frequency of client contact with the language and social institutions of both the dominant and heritage cultures.

This instrument is designed for use in a variety of client-practitioner relationships. The indicators are not only relevant across multiple cultures, they are generalizable and allow practitioners to alter the base indicators to best fit the client’s unique social-psychological context. However, while this instrument can be easily administered to adolescent and adult clients, it would not be optimal for use with child clients, as questions assume the client’s self-selection of media and friendship. All indicators contain brief and clear language and responses are on a 6-point Likert scale. Respondents can answer easily, answers can be standardized and compared over time, and additional variation in responses can be captured, as compared to using a 5- or 4-point, Likert scale. In designing this instrument, I assume that respondents have a cultural tie to a culture in which the dominant language is not English. Language has been shown by many researchers to be a powerful indicator of acculturation and, as I rely heavily on language in many of my indicators, the scale I have developed would lose value if used with a client who associates his or her non-US culture with the English language.

The Instrument: Thirty Indicators over Three Domains

Pre-Instrument Questions

The purpose of the pre-instrument questions is to acquire the ethnicity and language with which the client identifies herself. These questions give the client the opportunity to identify his ethnicity and language and prevent the practitioner from making assumptions about the client’s ethnicity and the language associated with that ethnicity.

What is your ethnicity? [d1] ___________________________________________________

(The response to this question will be referred to as X in later statements/questions.)

What language is generally associated with this ethnicity? ________________________

(The response to this question will be referred to as Y in later statements/questions.)

The Instrument: Domain 1: Language

            Indicators within the domain of language usage and fluency are consistently included in instruments designed to assess a respondent’s level of acculturation and are shown to be significant indicators of acculturation (Cuéllar, Arnold and Maldonado 1995; Cortés, Rogler and Malgady 1994; Félix-Ortiz, Newcomb and Myers 1994; Mendoza 1989; Stephenson 2000; Zea, Asner-Self, Birman and Buki 2003). Some measurements of acculturation only use indicators related to language (Kamo and Zhou 1994; Krause and Goldenhar 1992). This instrument measures language usage rather than language competence or language preference. The bilinear format assesses how English and Y are (or are not) integrated into the client’s life.

 

Not at all

Very Low

Low

Fair

High

Very High

Indicator 1: Rate your English language fluency (or how comfortable you are using the English language).

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 2: Rate your Y language fluency (or how comfortable you are using the Y language).

 

 

 

 

 

 

The pair of indicators above assesses the client’s self-perceived, overall language competence. English language fluency indicates client adaptation to the dominantUSculture, while Y language fluency indicates ties to the heritage culture. The following indicators in this domain assess actual language usage overall and in more specific situations.

All indicators below focus on frequency rather than subjective assessments of quality (such as, “How well do you speak English at school or work?”). Frequent use of the English language, a straight-forward, easily-answered question, has been shown to be a significant indicator of acculturation to the dominant culture, whereas the client’s perceived quality of language use in different scenarios is more indicative of acculturative stress than level of acculturation and is more fitting of the conversation that follows the client’s completion of the instrument when further reflection is required to grapple with the client’s perception of the acculturative process.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 3: How often do you speak English on a daily basis?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 4: How often do you speak Y on a daily basis?

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 5: How often do you speak English at school or work?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 6: How often do you speak Y on at school or work?

 

 

 

 

 

 

The indicators highlighting the client’s school or work environment can, in conjunction with the additional indicators in this domain, give the practitioner a sense of whether or not the client has opportunities at work or school to speak Y, whether or not the client is electively speaking or not speaking Y, and may hint at other constraints on language usage in the client’s environment. For example, if in all situations but school or work the client speaks Y, the practitioner may discuss this with the client to uncover what constrains Y usage at school or work. Speaking English at school or work may indicate client adaptation to the dominant culture, whereas speaking Y at school or work may indicate a linkage to his or her heritage culture.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 7: How often do you speak English with family?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 8: How often do you speak Y with family?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 7: How often do you speak English with US-American friends?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 8: How often do you speak Y with US-American friends?

 

 

 

 

 

 

These indicators highlights the client’s home environment and gives the practitioner a comparison point in terms of Y usage at home compared to at school or work. If English is often spoken at home, this would indicate another level of adaptation to the dominant culture, as home usage of English would often be a more voluntary usage of English.  Further indicators give the practitioner information about the respondent’s environment, which may be used in a discussion to assess the level to which the client’s US-American friends reside in the same community as the client or if US-American friends that either speak English or Y are being intentionally selected by the client for other reasons. If the respondent often speaks Y, rather than English, with US-American friends, this gives the practitioner information on the prominence or availability of the Y language and the X culture in their community.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 9: How often do you write in English during a typical day?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 10: How often do you write in Y during a typical day?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Writing in one language rather than another (or writing in both equally or in neither at all) gives the practitioner additional information on the client’s ties to his or her heritage culture as well as the client’s adaptation to the dominant culture.

Domain 2: Cultural Behavior

Similar to the domain of language, indicators within the domain of cultural behavior are also consistently included in instruments designed to assess the respondent’s level of acculturation (Cuéllar, Arnold and Maldonado 1995; Cortés, Rogler and Malgady 1994; Félix-Ortiz, Newcomb and Myers 1994; Mendoza 1989; Stephenson 2000). Behavior can often tell a practitioner more about a client’s level of acculturation than simply asking questions devoted to preference. Reporting on behavior requires less reflection from the client (and controls more for client subjectivity) than requesting information on preferences. Zea, Asner-Self, Birman, and Buki (2003) do not use indicators in the domain of cultural behavior in their instrument, claiming behavior is more a function of availability of media from the heritage culture than a function of preference. While a lack of availability may preclude the client’s participation in certain activities, participation in behaviors (such as consumption of media from the heritage or dominant culture) can be taken to indicate levels of acculturation in each culture, regardless of the client’s preferred behavior. Dissonance between a client’s actions and preferences is often [d2] indicative of acculturative stress, rather than level of acculturation.  Further, as Mendoza (1989) argues, increased contact with a non-heritage culture increases the likelihood of cultural change by virtue of frequency of contact alone, so behaviorally-focused indicators of acculturation are also salient forbearers of long-term cultural change.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 11: How often do you watch English-language television programs?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 12: How often do you watch Y-language television programs?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 13: How often do you watch English-language movies?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 14: How often do you watch Y-language movies?

 

 

 

 

 

 

The language used in a specific activity, whether that activity is television-watching, movie-going, or newspaper-reading, frequently involves the transmission of culture. A client’s regular participation in activities in either English or Y may indicate client behaviors that are imbued with dominant or heritage cultural content, respectively. Watching English-language television programming or Y may be seen as a way of selecting to receive information about the outside world.  Television programs, along with newspapers, may be seen as shaping and filtering an individual’s worldview, whereas movies, along with magazines, may be seen as auxiliary to the individual’s worldview. Watching movies rather than television in Y may indicate a stronger social, rather than cultural, connection to X than to a dominant (US) culture. In other words, social support or entertainment may occur in Y, while the client may more closely identify culturally with the dominant culture. It may also suggest the outcome of limited English language ability. English television- and movie-watching are both indicative of adaptations to the dominant culture.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 15: How often do you read English-language newspapers?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 16: How often do you reach Y-language newspapers?

 

 

 

 

 

    

Indicator 17: How often do you read English-language magazines?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 18: How often do you read Y-language magazines?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Reading English-language newspapers (rather than Y) again indicates the worldview being transmitted, specifically that the dominant culture is being transmitted. Magazines are often selected for entertainment value and may reflect social, rather than a cultural connection to a culture. Again, English newspaper- and magazine-reading are both indicative of some level of adaptation to the dominant culture.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 19: How often do you listen to English-language music?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 20: How often do you listen to Y-language music?

 

 

 

 

 

 

Domain 3: Cultural Knowledge

Cultural knowledge, which refers to knowledge of cultural referents, has been demonstrated to be a significant dimension of acculturation in studies such as those conducted by Félix-Ortiz, Newcomb, and Myers (1994) and Stephenson (2000). Zea, Asner-Self, Birman, and Buki (2003) suggest that cultural knowledge is indicative of cultural competence, or the capacity to function successfully in a specific culture, which is an important factor in assessing an individual’s level of acculturation.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 21: I know US-American national heroes.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 22: I know national heroes from X.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 23: I know US-American political leaders.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 24: I know political leaders from X.

 

 

 

 

 

 

These indicators assesses how familiar the client is with this aspect of theUSnational identity. Familiarity with national heroes may be understood as part of the socialization process of US-Americans and may be indicative of some level of client adaptation to the dominantUSculture. Familiarity with US-American political leaders can also be understood as part of the socialization process of US-Americans. Moreover, knowledge of political leaders may indicate an engagement in theUSpolitical process.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 25: I know popular US-American television shows.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 26: I know popular television shows in Y language.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Familiarity with popular television shows in the dominant or heritage culture (or both) indicates an understanding of the popular aspects of the culture. Such understanding may allow the client to participate in conversations with colleagues regarding a popular television show or magazine, and may allow the client to function more successfully and fluidly in the dominant culture, depending on the client’s environment and his or her actual interaction with the dominantUSculture. Similarly, familiarity with popular aspects of the heritage culture may allow the client to recognize popular heritage culture references with family or community, and may thereby allow for the creation of a social support network.  

In order to illustrate potential client-practitioner conversations that may follow administration, I will extrapolate on this further and  suggest that the client’s social support network in his or her heritage culture may further improve client ease in maneuvering in the dominant culture through, for instance, increased self-esteem through identification and participation with his or her heritage culture. Please note this extrapolation on the benefits of maintaining linkages to the heritage culture is beyond the scope of this instrument and may easily be applied to any of the indicators. I mention it here to highlight the bilinear, or bicultural, nature of these indicators, as well as the complexity of meaning possible in client responses depending on their environment and the level of dissonance between his or her heritage culture and the dominant culture.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 27: I know popular US-American newspapers or magazines.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 28: I know popular newspapers or magazines in Y language.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Familiarity with popular printed materials, in combination with frequent reading of English-language materials, may indicate not only the client’s regular consumption of printed media, but a client’s a confidence to recognize what is or is not popular in the dominant culture, both of which may be indicative of client adaptation to the dominant culture.

 

Not at all

Very little

A little

Somewhat

Much

Very much

Indicator 29: I know popular US-American actors and actresses.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Indicator 30: I know popular actors and actresses from X.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Familiarity with popular US actors and actresses, in combination with frequent viewing of English-language media, may similarly indicate the client’s regular consumption of such media and his or her confidence in recognizing the popularity of actors and actresses in the dominant culture.

Conclusion

People live in a complex social-psychological world and, in our post-industrial, globalized society, where transnationalism is common and multiculturalism is a norm (Bennett, Bennett and Landis 2004), the complexity and changing meaning of acculturation, as well as the varieties of ways individuals experience acculturation, must be recognized. The instrument presented here assesses acculturation in two cultures along multiple domains in order to capture the complex social-psychological context in which the acculturative process occurs. Yet this instrument should not be used in isolation. Considering the client in his or her environment, a generic instrument to measure levels of acculturation cannot be suitable for every client.

First, indicators should be developed with an understanding of the complexity of the client’s world and the necessity of flexible indicators that can be altered to the unique client situation. In-depth research and understanding of a client’s multifaceted, complex context is an important aspect of working with a multicultural client, as is recognition of the client’s impact on their environment and the ever-present person-situation dynamic that guides the acculturative process. An instrument that measures acculturation should not presuppose a cultural homeostasis, or view culture as a static or singular concept (Baptiste, Jr. 1993; Kottak 1999; Ward and Chang 1997). Second, when administering an instrument like this, practitioners must consider carefully the client’s context. They must think creatively about the multiple approaches to measuring acculturation and what can be learned from existing measures and relate models of acculturation to the practice of assessing client levels of acculturation. They must then apply findings with clients to the critical assessment of existing models, and understand which contextual factors may regulate or mediate the dynamic process of acculturation (Bennett, Bennett and Landis 2004; Berry 2003; Cabassa 2003).

Third, the practitioner should be aware of the potential ethnocentricity that may be found in both existing instruments and instruments that the practitioner creates. To this end, the practitioner should consciously avoid constructing a client’s heritage culture as a risk factor. In addition to researching the client’s context, the practitioner must do this in a culturally sensitive manner that maintains the dignity, value, and uniqueness of the client and his or her experience of both the dominant and his or her heritage culture. For example, the practitioner should be careful not to assume anything about the client’s heritage culture, including the language spoken and religion practiced. Further, in measuring acculturation, the practitioner must be conscious of the risk of viewing the dominant culture’s values as universally superior to conflicting values attributed to non-dominant cultures.  They must also keep in mind the risk that widespread negative biases and stereotypes pose to his or her construction of measures of acculturation and his or her work with the client (Greenfield 1994; Shelton et al. 2005; Zhou 2001). Finally, given a client’s responses to the indicators of acculturation and the critical thought the practitioner has given the client’s context, the instrument should frame a conversation with the client regarding his or her acculturative process, acculturative stress, ethnic identity, and other related or unrelated topics.

In social work practice, a practitioner’s awareness of a client’s level of acculturation enhances the practitioner’s cultural competence in interfacing with that client and deepens the practitioner’s understanding of how the client experiences her environment. As the practitioner strives for greater cultural competence and a better understanding of the client’s world, she is also protecting the client’s dignity by demonstrating respect for the client. A practitioner’s awareness of acculturation also increases her recognition of the impact of acculturation on other health, psychological, and behavior outcomes and health and social service utilization. A client’s level of acculturation impacts social work practice, so a practitioner’s awareness of acculturation improves her practice of social work.  

References

Abe-Kim, Jennifer,Okazaki, Sumie, and Goto, Sharon G. 2001. Unidimensional versus multidimensional approaches to the assessment of acculturation for Asian American populations. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 7(3): 233-246.

Adrados, Juan-Luis Recio. 1997. Acculturation: The broader view. Theoretical framework of the acculturation scales. Substance Use & Misuse 32(12-13): 1919-1924.

Baptiste, Jr., David A. 1993. Immigrant families, adolescents and acculturation: Insights for therapists. In Families on the move: Migration, Immigration, Emigration, and Mobility, eds Barbara H. Settles, Daniel E. Hanks III, and Marvin B. Sussman, 341-364. Binghamton,NY: TheHaworthPress.

Bennett, Janet, Bennett, Milton, and Landis, Dan. 2004. Introduction and overview. In Handbook of intercultural training, 3rd ed, eds Dan Landis, Janet M. Bennett, and Milton J. Bennett, 1-10.Thousand Oaks,CA: Sage.

Bennett, John W. 2005. The ecological transition: cultural anthropology and human adaptation. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.

Berry, John W. 2003. Conceptual approaches to acculturation. In Acculturation: Advances in theory, measurement, and applied research, eds Kevin M. Chun, Pamela Balls Organista, and Gerardo Marín, 17-37.Washington,DC: American Psychological Association.

Berry, John W. 1997. Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology: An International Review 46(1): 5-68.

Berry, John. 1980. Acculturation as varieties of adaptation. In Acculturation: Theory, models and some new findings, ed. Amado M. Padilla, 9-25.Washington, D.C: American Association for the Advancement of Science Selected Symposium.

Bidney, David. 1947. Human nature and the cultural process. American Anthropologist 49(3): 375-399.

Cabassa, Leopoldo J. 2003. Measuring acculturation: Where we are and where we need to go. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 25(2): 127-146.

Cortés, Dharma E., Rogler, Lloyd H., and Malgady, Robert G. 1994. Biculturality among Puerto Rican adults in the United States. Journal of Community Psychology 22(5): 707-721.

Cuéllar,Israel, Arnold, Bill, and Maldonado, Roberto. 1995. Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans-II: A revision of the original ARSMA scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 17(3): 275-304.

Félix-Ortiz, Maria, Newcomb, Michael D. and Myers, Hector. 1994. A multidimensional measure of cultural identity for Latino and Latinaadolescents. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 16(2): 99-115.

Finch, Brian Karl and Vega, William A.  2003. Acculturation stress, social support, and self-rated health among Latinos. Journal of Immigrant Health 5(3): 109-117.

Gordon, Milton Myron. 1964. Assimilation in American life: The role of race, religion, and national origins.New York:OxfordUniversity Press.

Greenfield, Patricia M. 1994.Independenceand interdependence as developmental scripts: Implications for theory, research, and practice. In Cross-cultural roots of minority child development, eds.Patricia MarksGreenfield and Rodney R. Cocking, 1-37.Hillsdale,NJ: LawrenceErlbaum Associates.

Handwerker, W. Penn.2002. Quick ethnography.Walnut Creek,CA:Altamira Press.

Kamo, Yoshinori and Zhou, Min. 1994. Living arrangements of elderly Chinese and Japanese in the United States. Journal of Marriage & Family 56(3): 544-558.

Kottak, Conrad P. 1999. The new ecological anthropology. American Anthropologist 101(1): 23-35.

Krause, Neal and Goldenhar, Linda M. 1992. Acculturation and psychological distress in three groups of elderly Hispanics. Journal of Gerontology 47(6): S279-S288.

Lee, Mee Sook, Crittenden, Kathleen S., and Yu, Elena. 1996. Social support and depression among elderly Korean immigrants in the United States. International Journal of Aging & Human Development 42(4): 313-327.

Lee, Soo-Kyung, Sobal, Jeffery, and Frongillo, Edward A. 2003. Comparison of models of acculturation: The case of Korean Americans. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 34(3): 282-296.

Lieber, Eli, Chin, Dorothy, Nihira, Kazuo, and Mink, Iris Tan. (2001). Holding on and letting go: Identity and acculturation among Chinese immigrants. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology 7(3): 247-273.

Mendoza, Richard H. 1989. An empirical scale to measure type and degree of acculturation in Mexican-American adolescents and adults. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology20(4): 372-385.

Newman, David M. 2010. Sociology: Exploring the architecture of everyday life. Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.

Nguyen, Huong H.,LawrenceA.Messé, and Gary E. Stollak. 1999. Toward a more complex understanding of acculturation and adjustment: Cultural involvements and psychosocial functioning in Vietnamese youth. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 30(1): 5-31.

Redfield, Robert, Linton, Ralph, and Herskovits, Melville J. 1936. Memorandum for the study of acculturation. American Anthropologist 38(1): 149-152.

Ryder, Andrew G., Alden, Lynn E., and Paulhus, Delroy L. (2000). Is acculturation unidimensional or bidimensional? A head-to-head comparison in the prediction of personality, self-construal, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79(1): 49-65.

Shelton, J. Nicole, Tiffany Yip, , Jacquelynne S Eccles,., Celina M. Chatman, Andrew J. Fuligni and Carol Wong. 2005. Ethnic identity as a buffer of psychological adjustment to stress. In Navigating the future: Social identity, coping, and life tasks, eds Geraldine Downey, Jacquelynne S. Eccles, and Celina M. Chatman, 96-113. NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Silverblatt, Art. 2004. Media as social institution. American Behavioral Scientist 48(1): 35-41.

Stephenson, Margaret. 2000. Development and validation of the Stephenson Multigroup Acculturation Scale (SMAS). Psychological Assessment 12(1): 77-88.

Tsai, Jeanne L. 2001. Cultural orientation of Hmong young adults. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment 3(3/4): 99-114.

–––., Yu-Wen Ying and Peter A. Lee. 2000. The meaning of “being Chinese” and “being American”: Variation among Chinese American young adults. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 31(3): 302-332.

Vega, William A., Kolody, Bohdan, Valle, Ramon, and Weir, Judy. 1991. Social networks, social support, and their relationship to depression among immigrant women. Human Organization 50(2): 154-162.

Ward, Colleen and Chang, Weining C. 1997. “Cultural fit”: A new perspective on personality and sojourner adjustment. International Journal of Intercultural Relation 21(4): 525-533.

–––. and Kennedy,Antony. 1993. Where’s the “culture” in cross-cultural transition?: Comparative studies of sojourner adjustment. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 24(2): 221-249.

Wolf, Eric R. 1984. Culture: Panacea or problem? American Antiquity 49(2): 393-400.

–––.. 1982. Europe and the people without history.Berkeley:University ofCalifornia Press.

Zea, Maria Cecilia, Asner-Self, Kimberly K., Birman, Dina, &Buki,LydiaP. 2003. The Abbreviated Multidimensional Acculturation Scale: Empirical validation with two Latino/Latina samples. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology 9(2): 107-126.

Zhou, Min. 2001. Straddling different worlds: The acculturation of Vietnamese refugee children. In Ethnicities: Children of immigrants in America, eds Rubén G. Rumbaut and Alejandro Portes, 187-227.Berkeley:UniversityofCaliforniaPress.


 [d1]I would also suggest setting this up with a sentence about what’s going on here and the role of these pre-questions.

 [d2]Is often?